Herpetology and Me

The Inside Story: Reptile ownership often begins with a baby Green Iguana

Saturday, March 17, 2012

Iguanas Use Mockingbird Calls To Survive


The Thoughtful Animal

The Thoughtful Animal

Exploring the evolution and architecture of the mind

The Thoughtful Animal Home

http://blogs.scientificamerican.com/thoughtful-animal/2012/03/15/eavesdropping-iguanas-use-mockingbird-calls-to-survive/avesdropping






Eavesdropping Iguanas use Mockingbirds' Calls to Survive

By Jason G. Goldman | March 15, 2012




Predator-prey interactions are often viewed as evolutionary arms races; while predators improve their hunting behaviors and their ability to sneak up on their prey, the prey improve upon their abilities to detect and escape from their predators. The problem, of course, is that there is a trade-off between maintaining vigilance – the attention necessary to be consistently aware of others in the environment takes quite a bit of physical and mental energy – and doing all the other things that an animal must do, such as finding its own food. As a result of this trade-off, many social species, especially mammalian and avian species, have developed alarm calls. Alarm calls are specific vocalizations that signal the presence of a danger in the environment to nearby conspecifics, and sometimes contain additional information about the type of threat or predator.

Subsequent to the introduction of predatory birds, howler monkeys on Barro Colorado Island near Panama rapidly developed an alarm call specific for those birds that indicated the presence of an avian predator: something like “danger from above!” They did not merely adapt an already existing alarm call to the new predator, they developed an entirely new one.

In certain cases, some species have developed the additional ability to eavesdrop on the alarm calls of other species, gaining access to an additional source of information relevant to the presence of danger in the environment. This ability could be the consequence of a learned association between the alarm calls of another species and the presence of the predator, or it could be due to certain auditory properties common to the alarm calls of both species. More research is required to tease apart these possibilities. However, until recently, it was thought that the ability to identify and react to the alarm calls of other species was only possible in species that already had vocal communication. Several years ago, however, researchers from Princeton University observed this behavior in an unlikely species – a non-vocal reptile called the Galapagos marine iguana (Amblyrhynchus cristatus).

Prior to this observation, it was thought that non-vocal species, who did not have alarm calls themselves, would not be able to associate complex auditory stimuli with the presence of a predator. The Galapagos marine iguana does not have any vocalizations. Instead, they communicate by using visual and olfactory signals. However, they live among the Galapagos mockingbirds (Mimus parvulus), a species that does have auditory vocalizations and specific alarm calls. Further, since iguanas primarily live on the rocky shoreline, they are often unable to view hawks (their main predator) until it is too late. If they had the ability to eavesdrop on the alarm calls of the mockingbirds, they would possibly be able to engage their anti-predator behaviors significantly earlier and increase their chances of survival.

The researchers recorded two types of vocalizations from the Galapagos mockingbirds: their song calls and their alarm calls, and edited them into soundtracks each containing two or three examples of either type of call. The researchers would find a cluster of juvenile and female iguanas (the hawks mostly ignore the mature males) in three different sites on the coast of Santa Fe Island, and would play back the various mockingbird calls through a portable speaker system.



Galapagos Mockingbird


Previous research indicated that upon detecting a hawk, iguanas who were aggregated in a cluster would scatter in different directions, like many prey animals do, perhaps in an attempt to confuse the predator. Upon playback, the researchers noted the behavior of the iguanas. Their behaviors were coded as “non-response,” “alert,” (head raise, orienting towards the sound), or “escape” (walking or running away).

Forty-five percent of iguanas showed vigilance behavior during the playback of the alarm call, compared with only 28.1% of iguanas during the playback of the mockingbird songs, a statistically significant difference. This suggests that the Galapagos iguanas are able to eavesdrop on the alarm calls of the mockingbirds and respond accordingly. However, in addition to the type of playback, the time of day and the data collection site were also significant factors in predicting the proportion of iguanas responding to the playbacks.




Proportion of iguanas responding, according to type of call and site.

At each site, the iguanas successfully differentiated between the alarm calls and the songs, responding with escape behaviors more often subsequent to the alarm call. This is the first experimental evidence that a non-vocal species associated the alarm call of a different species with the presence of a nearby predator.


Recording sites on the island, with the general hunting flight trajectory of the hawks indicated with an arrow.

What might account for the different responses among recording sites? The authors speculate that differences in ambient noise could have contributed, resulting in changes in volume or sound quality. As each recording site was on the rocky coast of the island, the sound quality would be subject to wind and the sounds of the ocean. An alternative explanation, however, is that there are true differences between the sites, perhaps owing to slight variations in predator behavior or mockingbird vocalizations. The fact that the hawks on Santa Fe island begin their hunts each day on the northern side of the island and proceed south throughout the day might indeed result in higher predation rates at site three. If the hawks are successful at site three on a given day, they might not have any reason to continue on to sites one or two.

It was also noted that the iguanas were more responsive to the playbacks earlier in the day. One possibility is that these cold-blooded reptiles are more responsive earlier in the day, when their body temperature is lower, and they therefore require more time to escape due to their reduced agility. As the day continues, the sun passes overhead, and their body heat increases, their ability to escape more quickly might improve.

It is particularly impressive that these iguanas appear able to capitalize on the alarm calls of another species, the Galapagos mockingbird. By considering the environmental constraints placed on these iguanas by their specific location on the coast of Santa Fe Island, the elegance of evolution and natural selection becomes apparent.

On Santa Fe Island, hawks routinely approach begin their day hunting in the north, and proceed south throughout the day. It might make sense, therefore, that the iguanas generally orient towards the north so that they would be able to see the hawks coming. However, marine iguanas such as these must orient their bodies directly towards the sun, or at 180 degree angles to it, in order to survive. Without this thermoregulation, they would quickly overheat and die. The realities of their environmental requirements are directly at odds: face north to anticipate predation and risk overheating, or face the sun to avoid overheating and risk being eaten by an unseen hawk. The ability to capitalize on the auditory alarm vocalizations of another species – especially a species less constrained by body temperature needs – could therefore provide significant benefit. The evolution of this ability would allow the iguanas to simultaneously maintain their body temperature while maintaining awareness of potential threats of predation.

In order to confirm this evolutionary explanation, more research would be required to identify whether naive Galapagos iguanas, never exposed to the threat of predation or the calls of the mockingbirds, would recognize and respond to the alarm calls. The other possibility is that this is simply the result of associative learning of complex auditory information. In either case, the eavesdropping ability of this non-vocal reptile species is remarkable. For these iguanas, it truly is a sin to kill a mockingbird.

Vitousek MN, Adelman JS, Gregory NC, & Clair JJ (2007). Heterospecific alarm call recognition in a non-vocal reptile. Biology letters, 3 (6), 632-634. PMID: 17911047

Images: Galapagos marine iguana via Flickr/Max xx, mockingbird via Smugmug/Jerry Fiddler.

About the Author: Jason G. Goldman is a graduate student in developmental psychology at the University of Southern California, where he studies the evolutionary and developmental origins of the mind in humans and non-human animals. Jason is also Psychology and Neuroscience Editor for ResearchBlogging.org and Editor of Open Lab 2010. He lives in Los Angeles, CA. Follow on Google+. Follow on Twitter @jgold85.



The views expressed are those of the author and are not necessarily those of Scientific American.



..

No comments:

Post a Comment